Monday, October 1, 2018

Environmental heterogeneity and phenotypic markers... (Clara B. Jones)



Environmental Heterogeneity and Phenotypic Markers Create Longer Distances Between Nodes

Life is uncertain. Life in lower income communities is more uncertain than the norm. This unpredictability will have profound consequences for the life historical features of the poor in American society where differential gender, race, and class may be permanent markers of social inferiority. It is tempting to speculate that the costs of conformity are not perceived by the compliant or convert, and psychological research suggests three categories of response that may lead individuals to commit errors of judgment in uncertain conditions. People may employ a “representative heuristic” when drawing conclusions impulsively, without rational methods of decision-making. On this basis, a model of the world—not necessarily a conscious one—is constructed which is more likely to value what has occurred in the past, decreasing the effect of new stimuli representing changes in the environment. If, as Shelby Steele stated, “The [non-immigrant African-American underclass] …is basically as free as he or she wants to be.”, an opinion with which I partially agree, most of these individuals may not view the world as Steele does because of historical factors.

My qualified support of Steele’s statement is based on the observations that initial conditions, and the events that succeed them, are dynamic. Some lives may spin out of control, take a chaotic path, unpredictably. Rigid conformity to norms of groups (e.g., the “black church,” Black Lives Matter, Nation of Islam) to which the individual is exposed may stabilize the environment, as Muzafer Sherif suggested more than 70 years ago. It is important to investigate whether the representative heuristic is more likely to be formed before, after, or during the process of compliance of conversion and to understand tactics and strategies that might be employed to weaken its constraints upon perception. For example, instruction in decision-making techniques might counteract representativeness.
Individuals may, also, misjudge the probabilities of events occurring in time and space. Judgments based on the underlying odds of phenomena occurring in communities of the underclasses may not accurately predict events in the social, economic, and political mainstream networks. Repeated experiences in an environment will be generalized to other environments and will lead to expectations about how others will behave. These processes, characteristic of all organisms with learning capacities, will function in the underclasses to foreclose the perception of opportunities where they exist outside their communities that are often segregated from non-marginal spaces. This resistance of learned beliefs, attitudes, values, and behaviors will contribute to a form of passive conformity through continuing reinforcement of the behaviors initially conditioned, and it may be difficult for the Panopticon Network to police marginal spaces.

Little is known about changing mind-sets, redrawing cognitive maps short of intensive behavioral modification (e.g., by psychotherapy, coercion, force). But, I believe that public schools and local governments can significantly influence the experiences of young members of the underclass in spaces outside their own proscribed communities, thus increasing the likelihood that they will make veridical decisions and, perhaps, decreasing the probability of conformity as a denial of and recomposition for the consequences of real and perceived victimization.

Errors in judgment may, also, result from the manner in which a problem is “framed.” Research has demonstrated that solutions to problems depend on the architecture of a statement. It has been found that the most rational decisions are made when statements are framed broadly rather than narrowly, and, it seems to me, that narrow framing is likely to favor conformity by restricting the pattern of events in the environment to which the individual responds. Alternative social tactics and strategies, then, would not, all other things being equal, be perceived, limiting cognitive flexibility.

Many years of my teaching career were spent at two “historically black colleges.” At one of these schools, a student of mine claimed that psychologists used the white rat for their laboratory studies because, “Psychology is a racist science.” The narrowness of this student’s statement led to errors in judgment, most likely strongly influenced by emotions, preventing her from imagining alternative explanations. Similar to expressing a representativeness heuristic and ignoring the underlying probabilities of events, narrow framing is the result of and has the ability to create perceptual biases, constraining our understanding of the world. My student was prevented from coding certain components of her environment in particular ways as if the limitations of her knowledge of events corresponded to richness of interpretation. This young woman was not unable to learn—to modify her behavior; but, learning was made more difficult because a stage of re-framing was required to adjust her perceptions in order to associate the genetic homogeneity of a white rat with its appropriateness as a relatively homogeneous lab control, rather than a symbol of historical domination.

Narrow framing may make a person more likely to conform to elements of the Panopticon Network by creating a relatively undifferentiated set of stimuli with which events are endowed with meaning. Fewer environmental cues, then, will be available for association and identification. If conformity is a type of social influence, then individuals who frame narrowly are more susceptible to incitement because fewer stimuli will evoke their conforming (cooperative?) behavior. Coherent belief systems may attract many citizens in societies, but those on the margins who frame narrowly may be especially resistant to potentially life-enhancing social influence.


No comments:

Post a Comment