Knowledge
and Power
Charles
Darwin subordinated God, trapped Him in a corner of history furnished
with adaptation and selection—change through time both fast and
slow. Mostly, like generations of slaves picking cotton on the red
clay soil of Mississippi, slow to breathe the fast air of the
European world in Natchez. Would Darwin have watched the blacks as
he watched finches in Galapagos, fit to their own southern habitats,
trapped in their own struggles to survive a change in climate, in
kin, in friends?
John
Hurrell l Crook stated, “Social behavior is a subterfuge.” The
myth of sociality is as ancient as a Nubian kingdom’s secrets. The
slave is bound by chemistry and time to serve herself. Thus are the
master’s interests opposed to his subordinates’ as Darwin’s
were to God’s. The world’s Pareto Optimum is a formula for
social stasis, slow as slaves moved on red clay…slow as the kings
of Nubia moved through history.
Definitions
are relative to criteria defining the parameters of particular
domains or events or phenomena. In the case of offspring quality,
criteria depend upon the particular cultural standards of success.
Are children in Harlem to be judged by the norms of the subordinate
African-American culture or the dominant European-American culture?
By definition, the success of all children in American society is
determined by dominant folkways, European and technological, and a
challenge to any alternate group to assimilate, to mimic, to
condition themselves to the dominant paradigms. The black child in
Harlem or Oakland may be a good person by Judeo-Christian standards,
but she may not be acceptable by other standards of Western European
and/or American society. Certain African-American interest groups
may consider this unfair or racist; but, it is simply a byproduct of
the architecture of power relations throughout history. If blacks
assume the posture of victims until European society incorporates
their standards (“ebonics”, welfare, “affirmative action,”
dysfunctional/disorganized families, etc.), the conflicts of interest
between the two cultures will likely intensify. Non-European
standards represent a tax on European society, a tax that will
ultimately incur more costs than benefits for both cultural regimes.
Many
hypotheses have been proposed to account for the status of social
inferiors (non-immigrant Afro-Americans, women, children,
homosexuals, etc.) in American society. Of necessity, these
hypotheses entail endogenous factors, exogenous factors, or factors
produced by an interaction between these stimuli. Three
non-evolutionary hypotheses accounting for the behavior of social
inferiors with reference to environmental causes are particularly
persuasive. These are “learned helplessness”, “resentful
demoralization”, and “social phobia”.
a.
“Learned Helplessness”
A
large literature exists on Seligman’s “learned helplessness,”
and my application of the mechanism to the condition of social
subordinates, in particular, blacks and women, is not unique. It was
clear to researchers who first identified learned helplessness in
canines that the concept may apply under stress or weakened defenses
to other taxa, including humans. Dogs in an experimental situation
were shocked electrically without a means of escape. Having learned
that escape is impossible, the experimental dogs gave up trying to
escape, becoming “helpless” in a manner preventing their
detecting a change in the experimental condition leading to avoidance
of shock.
Following
the arguments of researchers in the field of “learned
helplessness,” that condition may be more apparent than real. Just
as some non-immigrant Afro-Americans may learn to associate “success”
(reward) with precocious pregnancy, impulsive aggression, drugs, and
rap music, so might they learn to associate success with delayed
gratification, individual responsibility, and individual principle.
If early experience teaches that the environment is hostile,
unpredictable, or uncontrollable, then the child may generalize this
mind-set or conditioned responses to other situations, making social
inferiority likely. The child’s early environment may construct a
cognitive map of a world in which powerlessness is not only a
function of her racial or sexual value in society but, also, is a
function of her role in the personal domain of family which may
condition impotence, in part, by failing to teach behavior reflecting
morals, ethics, character, and, perhaps most important, self-worth.
A lesson for humans of the “learned helplessness” experiments is
that a concordance exists between perceived self-potency and freedom.
While the environment may be like a network with rewards of varying
value at only a proportion of its nodes, the individual can learn to
combine and recombine rewarded pathways into a lifetime strategy
minimizing self-abuse and abuse of others, maximizing individual
accountability.
b.
“Resentful Demoralization”
Another
psychological mechanism that may explain the social inferiority of
some groups is “resentful demoralization” whereby individuals in
one group become less productive, less efficient, or less motivated
than they might have been because of resentment stimulated by
perceived or real special treatment received by another group. This
state may be more apparent than real, emphasizing the value of
teaching individuals rational and conscious problem-solving skills,
as well as, statistical thinking, to measure the difference.
“Resentful demoralization” would seem to render the individual
more vulnerable to manipulation by others, contrary to the
individual’s self-interests. If social inferiors can enhance their
status by a more realistic and less resentful interpretation of
reality, perhaps retraining could revise cognitive maps or
conditioned responses in such a manner that alternative tactics and
strategies might be envisioned. This suggestion implies that
cognitive or conditioned mastery-training or some other form of
psychological, especially, behavioral, immunization might combat both
“resentful demoralization” and “learned helplessness.”
c.
“Social Phobia”
A
third explanation for social inferiority might be “social phobia”
in which the individual fears evaluation by others in all its forms.
Persons with this phobia avoid anxiety-producing situations or
tolerate anxiety-producing situations with significant discomfort.
“Social phobias” are thought to arise by classical conditioning
during which reflexes (including emotional responses) are paired with
new stimuli (such as the stimuli in a social situation inducing
anxiety). Responses to stimuli may permeate an individual’s social
life through the process of stimulus generalization whereby the
individual responds similarly to similar stimuli. While phobias may
sometimes be overcome by a number of therapeutic techniques (e.g.,
“systematic desensitization”), learned behaviors are difficult to
change once consolidated, as Skinner pointed out.
“Learned
helplessness,” “resentful demoralization,” and “social
phobias,” if unchecked, may lead to obsessions and/or compulsions
about the individual’s social inferiority, further inhibiting the
likelihood of change. Non-immigrant African-Americans, for example,
can become fixated on race, skewing their perceptions of the world
and their roles in it. These and other fixations can lead to an
overestimation as well as an underestimation of her abilities
relative to the rigors of the environment. Such behaviors may lead
to responses varying in severity such as the rigid, authoritarian
parenting styles* common among non-immigrant African-American
families or the rigid adherence to the black church. Such obsessions
and compulsions reduce the individual’s anxiety in the face of
discomfort but reinforce the individual's social incompetence in the
dominant society.
*See
classic research by Pinderhughes EE, et al. (2000) Discipline responses: influences of parents' socioeconomic status, ethnicity, beliefs about parenting, stress, and cognitive-emotional processes. J Fam Psychol 14(3): 380-400.
No comments:
Post a Comment