Networks and Academia
As
a Biopsychologist trained in Ethology, Behavioral Ecology, and Behaviorism, my academic work has stressed the biological basis
of behavior, in particular, the evolution of social* behavior and
social organization, studied with the tools and questions of
behavioral ecology. For each behavioral pattern, one might ask, “Are
these responses likely to benefit or be deleterious to the actor’s
lifetime survival and/or reproductive success and inclusive
fitness?”. Unlike many behavioral scientists, I am not an extreme
environmentalist. Parsimony, alone, would lead to the conclusion
that behavior and social organization result from interaction
between/among endogenous and exogenous factors [G x E] and that the relative
contribution of each will vary with situation or context, anatomy, phylogeny, neural networks, physiology, stochasticity, and,
in my opinion, most importantly, genotype.
The
evidence for intrinsic [neural, physiological (e.g., hormonal) and
genetic] effects as inducers of behavior exists in two domains.
First, temperament, the “physical” basis of personality,
differentiates children from birth and may affect mood, responses to
environmental stimuli, and metabolism. In general, according to
empirical research, non-immigrant African-American neonates
(children?) exhibit more heightened “excitability” and precocious
motor development than Caucasians, traits that may interfere with
cognitive development if they inhibit processes such as thresholds of
sensation, attention, and perception, precursors to learning, memory,
language, thinking, and problem-solving. Related to this, Darwin
argued that human emotions, phenomena that may interact with
cognitive processes, express ancient behavioral tendencies,
reservoirs of our evolutionary past (also, see I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt,
one of my post-doctoral advisors).
The
second intrinsic trait that I find compelling as a possible
explanatory mechanism for some component of the behavioral
differences between non-immigrant African-Americans and Caucasians is
the existence of differential “phenotypic plasticity”
(or, flexibility), the variable extents to which organisms respond to environmental
change, including, perturbation[s]. The optimal response to heterogeneous conditions may be
“tracking” the environment or resistance to its differences, its
variations, and each biological strategy has its costs and benefits
relative to context. For the phenotype as a whole, the individual is
hypothesized to exhibit a “reaction norm” across all possible
environments, but the theoretically optimal response sets (e.g., dedicated,networks) may or may not be achieved due to maintenance costs such as the costs of learning or the upkeep of fine-tuned perceptual systems (e.g., for the detection
of change and the prevention of cheating by other organisms). While
little is known about the biological basis of behavioral plasticity
in mammals (but see literature on neural switching), future research
in the field may yield some understanding of intrinsic differences
among individuals and groups. IF non-immigrant African-Americans are
less “flexible” or, “plastic” than Caucasians, ON AVERAGE,
they may be at a disadvantage when environments change, at least, when they change at certain rates or frequencies.
The
“norms of reaction” of non-immigrant African-Americans and
Caucasians (or men and women**, etc.) to environmental stimuli may
differ, ON AVERAGE, and the environment may disfavor the responses of
individuals and groups defined by race or sex, respectfully,
conditional upon situation/context. Research on behavioral
plasticity in humans and other primates would facilitate our
understanding of reaction norms in humans and the costs and benefits
(e.g., in time and energy) associated with these. An adaptationist
program (e.g., Social Biology) would extend these studies to
hypotheses concerning differential reproduction and resilience of genotypes and the
evolution of alternative social strategies in different environmental regimes. While ethical considerations might prevent direct
experimental testing of the hypotheses, evidence would be obtained
from natural “experiments”, physiological and case studies and other quantitative work [in particular, simulations and the conduct of experiments using agent-based (individual-based)
modeling]. If the average performance of non-immigrant
African-Americans in US society is to some degree a function of
intrinsic factors, one is led to speculate that non-immigrant
African-American (and, for sex comparisons, female*) genetic and
physiological networks are more canalized (more conservative and or
“hardwired” biologically) than those of Caucasian-Americans or other ethnic or racial groups (or,
for sex comparisons, females may be more canalized than males).
The
discussion of academic networks and their relationship to Panopticon
Networks requires assessment of differential quality [relative to situation], survival, and
reproduction of genotypes determining inclusive fitness and the
individual’s assessment of her advantages and disadvantages. An
increasing body of research suggests that one process may be labeled
“phenotype matching” (“greenbeards”) whereby individuals copy
or imitate or model, consciously or otherwise, hard-wired or
otherwise, the responses of conspecifics who are like themselves.
Psychologists and primatologists claim that few taxa are capable of
imitation; however, phenotypic matching does not necessarily rely
upon conscious and aware processes. Phenotypic matching [a type of "social learning"]is a
mechanism whereby organisms may maximize their chances of favoring themselves and genotypes like their own to the extent that phenotype and genotype
are correlated (“genetic accommodation”).
Observational
learning (“social learning”—where, in this instance, “social”
is employed in the sense of the Social Sciences cum
“interindividual interactions”) theory, a cognitive learning
paradigm, may help to explain how individuals “match” (see
Herrnstein's classic work) their responses to those of others. The
capacity
for observational learning will be intrinsic (hard-wired) and entails
observing a model (e.g., another individual’s phenotype) to
increase the likelihood of new responses that might be applied to new
situations (a type of response flexibility or plasticity***). The
efficacy of observational learning depends upon attention and memory,
properties of the neural system that are generally-agreed to be
hard-wired to a significant degree and that may differ among
individuals and groups [and that, in all likelihood, are responsive
to Gene x Environment (G x E) interactions]. Thus, although the
connections between stimuli and responses are not themselves wholly
intrinsic in all cases, individuals may respond differentially to abiotic and biotic, as well as, endogenous and exogenous social and non-social temporal, spatial, and
energetic events.
*In
this instance, I use the term, “social,” in the sense of W.E.
Hamilton (1964), whereby, “social” is limited to cooperation and
altruism.
**In
experimental genetics [see, for example, classic work by M. Lerner on "genetic homeostasis"], females have been found to be more
“canalized” than males, ON AVERAGE. Are non-immigrant
African-Americans more canalized, ON AVERAGE, than
Caucasian-Americans or other ethnic or racial groups [as determined by, say, SNP analyses/methods]? There are bound to be genetic differences between men & women, ON AVERAGE, and among the races, ON AVERAGE, because there will be variability, ON AVERAGE, in genetic architecture[s]. What these differences may be, ON AVERAGE, remain to be seen in most cases other than, say, body size, skin color, hair texture. There are not sufficient comparative genetic data for most traits. In order to determine biological differences x sex, gender, race, class or other factors, it will be necessary to investigate gene regulation in addition to brain structure & cognitive function. The importance of G x E interactions is understood.
***It
is conventional to consider “flexible” responses to be
reversible, while it is conventional to consider “plastic”
responses to be non-reversible. In her tome on developmental
plasticity, West-Eberhard does not make this distinction.
No comments:
Post a Comment